The psychology behind irrational decisions – Sara Garofalo

Alphabets Sounds Video

share us on:

The lesson explores the psychology behind irrational decision-making, particularly through the lens of loss aversion and cognitive biases. It illustrates how people often make seemingly illogical choices based on emotional responses to potential losses rather than rational evaluations of risk and reward. By understanding heuristics—mental shortcuts that can lead to errors in judgment—individuals can become more aware of their decision-making processes and improve their choices in complex situations.

The Psychology Behind Irrational Decisions

Imagine you’re on a game show. You’ve already won $1,000 in the first round and now have a chance to earn more. You can either take a guaranteed $500 bonus or flip a coin. If it lands on heads, you win an additional $1,000, but if it’s tails, you get nothing.

In the next round, you’ve earned $2,000 but face a penalty. You can either accept a $500 loss or flip a coin. Heads means you lose nothing, but tails results in a $1,000 loss.

Most people would choose the guaranteed bonus in the first round and opt for the coin flip in the second. This choice seems irrational because the odds and outcomes are the same in both rounds. So, why does the second scenario feel riskier?

Understanding Loss Aversion

The answer lies in a concept called loss aversion. According to rational economic theory, our decisions should be based on a mathematical evaluation of risk versus reward. However, research shows that the pain of losing something is psychologically about twice as powerful as the pleasure of gaining the same thing.

Loss aversion is a type of cognitive bias that stems from heuristics—mental shortcuts based on past experiences and intuition rather than detailed analysis. These shortcuts can lead to irrational decisions, not unlike falling in love or taking a leap off a cliff, but in the form of logical errors that are easily debunked.

The Challenge of Probability

Heuristics often falter in situations involving probability. Consider a die with four green faces and two red faces. If you roll it twenty times, which sequence would you bet on to win $25? In one study, 65% of college students chose a longer sequence over a shorter one, even though the shorter sequence was more likely. This is known as the conjunction fallacy, where our expectations mislead us into choosing the less probable option.

Heuristics and Numbers

Heuristics also struggle with numbers. In an experiment, students were asked if Mahatma Gandhi died before or after age 9 or 140. Despite these numbers being obviously incorrect, when asked to guess his actual age at death, the first group averaged 50, while the second averaged 67. This demonstrates the anchoring effect, where initial, irrelevant information skews our judgments. This effect is frequently exploited in marketing and negotiations to influence perceived value.

The Role of Heuristics

Despite their flaws, heuristics are valuable. Throughout human history, quick decision-making with limited information was crucial for survival. When time is short, heuristics can be life-saving. However, today’s complex world demands more nuanced decision-making, and unconscious biases can significantly impact areas like health, education, finance, and criminal justice.

Becoming Aware of Heuristics

While we can’t simply turn off our brain’s reliance on heuristics, we can become more aware of them. When faced with decisions involving numbers, probabilities, or multiple factors, take a moment to reflect. The intuitive choice might not always be the best one.

  1. Reflect on a recent decision you made that might have been influenced by loss aversion. How did the fear of loss impact your choice, and would you make the same decision knowing what you know now?
  2. Consider a time when you relied on heuristics for a quick decision. How did that decision turn out, and what does it reveal about the role of mental shortcuts in your life?
  3. Think about a situation where you faced a probability-based decision. How did your understanding of probability influence your choice, and what might you do differently in the future?
  4. Have you ever experienced the anchoring effect in a negotiation or purchase? How did initial information shape your perception, and how can you guard against this bias in the future?
  5. Discuss a scenario where you felt your decision was irrational. What cognitive biases might have been at play, and how can awareness of these biases help you make better decisions?
  6. Reflect on how heuristics have been both beneficial and detrimental in your decision-making process. Can you identify specific instances where they helped or hindered you?
  7. How do you balance the need for quick decision-making with the potential pitfalls of relying on heuristics? What strategies do you use to ensure more rational choices?
  8. In what ways can becoming more aware of cognitive biases and heuristics improve your decision-making in areas like finance, health, or education?
  1. Role-Playing Game Show Scenarios

    Engage in a role-playing activity where you simulate the game show scenarios described in the article. Work in pairs or small groups to discuss and decide on your choices for each round. Reflect on your decisions and identify any cognitive biases that influenced your choices.

  2. Loss Aversion Experiment

    Conduct a simple experiment to experience loss aversion firsthand. Use a set of hypothetical financial decisions involving gains and losses. Record your choices and analyze how loss aversion influenced your decision-making process. Share your findings with the class.

  3. Probability and Heuristics Workshop

    Participate in a workshop focused on understanding probability and heuristics. Solve probability puzzles and discuss how heuristics can lead to errors like the conjunction fallacy. Work in groups to identify strategies to mitigate these biases in real-life decision-making.

  4. Anchoring Effect Demonstration

    Experience the anchoring effect through a classroom demonstration. Respond to a series of questions with deliberately misleading anchors. Analyze how these anchors influenced your answers and discuss ways to recognize and counteract anchoring in everyday situations.

  5. Heuristics in History and Modern Life

    Research and present on the role of heuristics in historical decision-making and their impact on modern life. Explore how heuristics have been both beneficial and detrimental in various contexts. Present your findings to the class, highlighting key insights and lessons learned.

Let’s say you’re on a game show. You’ve already earned $1,000 in the first round when you land on the bonus space. Now, you have a choice: you can either take a $500 bonus guarantee or flip a coin. If it’s heads, you win a $1,000 bonus; if it’s tails, you get no bonus at all.

In the second round, you’ve earned $2,000 when you land on the penalty space. Now you have another choice: you can either take a $500 loss or try your luck at the coin flip. If it’s heads, you lose nothing; but if it’s tails, you lose $1,000 instead.

If you’re like most people, you probably chose to take the guaranteed bonus in the first round and flip the coin in the second round. But if you think about it, this makes no sense. The odds and outcomes in both rounds are exactly the same. So why does the second round seem much scarier?

The answer lies in a phenomenon known as loss aversion. Under rational economic theory, our decisions should follow a simple mathematical equation that weighs the level of risk against the amount at stake. However, studies have found that for many people, the negative psychological impact of losing something is about twice as strong as the positive impact of gaining the same thing.

Loss aversion is one cognitive bias that arises from heuristics—problem-solving approaches based on previous experience and intuition rather than careful analysis. These mental shortcuts can lead to irrational decisions, not like falling in love or bungee jumping off a cliff, but logical fallacies that can easily be proven wrong.

Situations involving probability are notoriously challenging for applying heuristics. For instance, say you were to roll a die with four green faces and two red faces twenty times. You can choose one of the following sequences of rolls, and if it shows up, you’ll win $25. Which would you pick? In one study, 65% of participants, all college students, chose sequence B, even though A is shorter and contained within B—in other words, more likely. This is what’s called a conjunction fallacy. Here, we expect to see more green rolls, so our brains can trick us into picking the less likely option.

Heuristics are also poor at dealing with numbers in general. In one example, students were split into two groups. The first group was asked whether Mahatma Gandhi died before or after age 9, while the second was asked whether he died before or after age 140. Both numbers were obviously way off, but when the students were then asked to guess the actual age at which he died, the first group’s answers averaged to 50, while the second group’s averaged to 67. Even though the clearly wrong information in the initial questions should have been irrelevant, it still affected the students’ estimates. This is an example of the anchoring effect, and it’s often used in marketing and negotiations to raise the prices that people are willing to pay.

So, if heuristics lead to all these wrong decisions, why do we even have them? Well, because they can be quite effective. For most of human history, survival depended on making quick decisions with limited information. When there’s no time to logically analyze all the possibilities, heuristics can sometimes save our lives. But today’s environment requires far more complex decision-making, and these decisions are more biased by unconscious factors than we think, affecting everything from health and education to finance and criminal justice.

We can’t just shut off our brain’s heuristics, but we can learn to be aware of them. When you come to a situation involving numbers, probability, or multiple details, pause for a second and consider that the intuitive answer might not be the right one after all.

IrrationalNot based on reason or logic; lacking sound judgment. – Despite the overwhelming evidence, his irrational fear of flying prevented him from attending the international conference.

DecisionsChoices made after considering different possibilities and outcomes. – The psychology course emphasized the importance of making informed decisions based on critical analysis of available data.

LossThe state of no longer having something or the experience of losing something valuable. – In behavioral economics, the concept of loss is crucial in understanding why people often make risk-averse choices.

AversionA strong dislike or disinclination towards something. – Her aversion to ambiguity made it difficult for her to embrace the uncertain outcomes of the experiment.

HeuristicsMental shortcuts or rules of thumb that simplify decision-making processes. – While heuristics can speed up decision-making, they can also lead to systematic errors in judgment.

ProbabilityThe measure of the likelihood that an event will occur. – Understanding probability is essential for evaluating the risks and benefits of different psychological interventions.

CognitiveRelated to mental processes such as perception, memory, and reasoning. – Cognitive biases can significantly influence how we interpret information and make decisions.

BiasA tendency to favor certain outcomes or interpretations over others, often in an unfair manner. – The study aimed to identify the bias in media reporting and its impact on public opinion.

AnchoringThe cognitive bias of relying too heavily on the first piece of information encountered when making decisions. – During negotiations, the initial offer often serves as an anchor, influencing the final agreement.

AwarenessThe state of being conscious of something, especially through direct experience or knowledge. – Increasing awareness of cognitive biases can help individuals make more rational decisions.

All Video Lessons

Login your account

Please login your account to get started.

Don't have an account?

Register your account

Please sign up your account to get started.

Already have an account?