Would you sacrifice one person to save five? – Eleanor Nelsen

Alphabets Sounds Video

share us on:

The lesson explores the ethical dilemma known as the trolley problem, which poses the question of whether one should sacrifice one person to save five others. It highlights the contrast between utilitarianism, which advocates for maximizing overall well-being, and the emotional and moral aversions people have towards directly causing harm. The discussion extends to real-world implications, such as decision-making in autonomous vehicles and military drones, emphasizing the importance of integrating ethical considerations into technology and policy.

Would You Sacrifice One Person to Save Five?

Imagine you’re standing by a railway track, and you see a trolley speeding uncontrollably towards five workers who can’t escape. You’re next to a lever that can divert the trolley onto another track, but there’s a catch: a single worker is on that track. What would you do? Would you choose to sacrifice one person to save five others? This scenario is known as the trolley problem, an ethical dilemma introduced by philosopher Philippa Foot in 1967. It challenges us to consider how we make decisions when faced with difficult choices. Should we aim for the best possible outcome, or adhere to a moral code that forbids causing harm?

In a survey, about 90% of people said they would pull the lever, sacrificing one to save five. This decision aligns with the principle of utilitarianism, which suggests that the morally right choice is the one that maximizes overall well-being. In this view, saving five lives is more important than the loss of one, even if it means actively causing someone’s death.

Exploring Different Perspectives

However, not everyone agrees with the utilitarian perspective, especially when the scenario changes slightly. Imagine you’re on a bridge overlooking the track, and the trolley is approaching. There’s no second track, but a large person stands next to you. If you push them onto the track, their body would stop the trolley, saving the five workers, but they would die. Utilitarians might argue the decision remains the same: sacrifice one to save five. Yet, only about 10% of people find it acceptable to push the person onto the tracks.

This difference highlights our instinctive belief that directly causing someone’s death is different from allowing them to die as a side effect. It feels inherently wrong, though it’s hard to articulate why. This intersection of ethics and psychology makes the trolley problem fascinating. It shows that our sense of right and wrong is influenced by more than just logical reasoning. For instance, men are more likely than women to say it’s acceptable to push the person off the bridge. Similarly, people who watch a comedy clip before the thought experiment are more inclined to make that choice. In a virtual reality study, participants were more willing to sacrifice men than women.

The Role of Emotions and the Brain

Researchers have examined brain activity in people considering both the classic and bridge versions of the dilemma. Both scenarios engage brain areas involved in decision-making and emotional responses. However, the bridge scenario triggers a stronger emotional reaction and increased activity in brain regions associated with internal conflict. Why is there a difference? One explanation is that pushing someone to their death feels more personal, triggering an emotional aversion to killing, even though logically, it might seem like the right choice.

The Real-World Implications

“Trolleyology” has faced criticism from some philosophers and psychologists who argue that the scenarios are too unrealistic for participants to take seriously. Yet, with advancements in technology, these ethical analyses are becoming increasingly relevant. For example, driverless cars may need to make decisions like causing a minor accident to prevent a major one. Similarly, governments are exploring autonomous military drones that might have to decide whether to risk civilian casualties to target a high-value threat.

To ensure these actions are ethical, we must determine in advance how to value human life and assess the greater good. Researchers working on autonomous systems are collaborating with philosophers to tackle the complex challenge of programming ethics into machines. This collaboration demonstrates that even hypothetical dilemmas can intersect with real-world issues, making ethical considerations more crucial than ever.

  1. Reflect on your initial reaction to the trolley problem. How did you feel about the decision to pull the lever, and what factors influenced your choice?
  2. Consider the difference in responses between the classic trolley problem and the bridge scenario. Why do you think people react differently to these situations?
  3. How do you personally reconcile the conflict between utilitarian principles and the emotional aversion to directly causing harm?
  4. Discuss how cultural, social, or personal values might influence one’s decision in the trolley problem. Can you identify any specific values that guide your own decision-making?
  5. In what ways do you think emotions play a role in ethical decision-making, especially in high-stakes scenarios like the trolley problem?
  6. How do you perceive the role of technology, such as autonomous vehicles or drones, in making ethical decisions? What concerns or hopes do you have about this intersection?
  7. Reflect on a real-life situation where you had to make a difficult ethical decision. How did you approach it, and what did you learn from the experience?
  8. Consider the collaboration between researchers and philosophers in programming ethics into machines. What challenges do you foresee in this endeavor, and how might they be addressed?
  1. Debate the Trolley Problem

    Engage in a structured debate with your classmates. Divide into groups representing different ethical perspectives, such as utilitarianism and deontological ethics. Argue your position on the trolley problem and explore the implications of each viewpoint. This will help you understand the complexity of ethical decision-making.

  2. Role-Playing Scenarios

    Participate in role-playing exercises where you are placed in various ethical dilemmas similar to the trolley problem. Reflect on your decisions and discuss with peers how emotions and logic influenced your choices. This activity will enhance your empathy and critical thinking skills.

  3. Analyze Real-World Applications

    Research and present on real-world scenarios where ethical dilemmas similar to the trolley problem arise, such as in autonomous vehicle programming or military drone operations. Discuss the ethical frameworks used in these situations and propose your solutions. This will connect theoretical ethics to practical applications.

  4. Brain Activity Exploration

    Study the neurological aspects of decision-making in ethical dilemmas by reviewing research on brain activity during the trolley problem. Present your findings on how different parts of the brain are activated and discuss the role of emotions in ethical reasoning. This will deepen your understanding of the psychological factors in ethics.

  5. Create a Survey

    Design and conduct a survey among your peers to gather data on their responses to various trolley problem scenarios. Analyze the results to identify trends and differences in ethical reasoning. Share your analysis with the class to foster a discussion on the diversity of ethical perspectives.

Imagine you’re watching a runaway trolley barreling down the tracks straight towards five workers who can’t escape. You happen to be standing next to a switch that will divert the trolley onto a second track. Here’s the problem: that track has a worker on it too, but just one. What do you do? Do you sacrifice one person to save five? This is the trolley problem, a version of an ethical dilemma that philosopher Philippa Foot devised in 1967. It’s popular because it forces us to think about how to choose when there are no good choices. Do we pick the action with the best outcome or stick to a moral code that prohibits causing someone’s death?

In one survey, about 90% of respondents said that it’s acceptable to flip the switch, letting one worker die to save five. Other studies, including a virtual reality simulation of the dilemma, have found similar results. These judgments align with the philosophical principle of utilitarianism, which argues that the morally correct decision is the one that maximizes well-being for the greatest number of people. The five lives outweigh one, even if achieving that outcome requires condemning someone to death.

However, people don’t always take the utilitarian view, which we can see by changing the trolley problem slightly. This time, you’re standing on a bridge over the track as the runaway trolley approaches. Now there’s no second track, but there is a very large person on the bridge next to you. If you push them over, their body will stop the trolley, saving the five workers, but they’ll die. To utilitarians, the decision is the same: lose one life to save five. But in this case, only about 10% of people say that it’s acceptable to push the person onto the tracks.

Our instincts tell us that deliberately causing someone’s death is different from allowing them to die as collateral damage. It just feels wrong for reasons that are hard to explain. This intersection between ethics and psychology is what’s so interesting about the trolley problem. The dilemma in its many variations reveals that what we think is right or wrong depends on factors other than a logical weighing of the pros and cons. For example, men are more likely than women to say it’s acceptable to push the person over the bridge. So are people who watch a comedy clip before doing the thought experiment. In one virtual reality study, people were more willing to sacrifice men than women.

Researchers have studied the brain activity of people thinking through the classic and bridge versions of the dilemma. Both scenarios activate areas of the brain involved in conscious decision-making and emotional responses. However, in the bridge version, the emotional response is much stronger, as is activity in an area of the brain associated with processing internal conflict. Why the difference? One explanation is that pushing someone to their death feels more personal, activating an emotional aversion to killing another person, but we feel conflicted because we know it’s still the logical choice.

“Trolleyology” has been criticized by some philosophers and psychologists. They argue that it doesn’t reveal anything because its premise is so unrealistic that study participants don’t take it seriously. But new technology is making this kind of ethical analysis more important than ever. For example, driverless cars may have to handle choices like causing a small accident to prevent a larger one. Meanwhile, governments are researching autonomous military drones that could end up making decisions about whether to risk civilian casualties to attack a high-value target.

If we want these actions to be ethical, we have to decide in advance how to value human life and judge the greater good. Researchers who study autonomous systems are collaborating with philosophers to address the complex problem of programming ethics into machines, which goes to show that even hypothetical dilemmas can wind up on a collision course with the real world.

SacrificeThe act of giving up something valued for the sake of other considerations or to benefit others. – In ethical discussions, sacrifice often involves weighing personal loss against the greater good.

TrolleyA thought experiment in ethics and psychology that explores moral dilemmas and decision-making. – The trolley problem challenges individuals to consider the implications of their choices in life-and-death scenarios.

UtilitarianismA philosophical theory that suggests the best action is the one that maximizes overall happiness or utility. – Utilitarianism often guides ethical decision-making by prioritizing actions that benefit the majority.

EthicsThe branch of philosophy that deals with questions of morality and the principles of right and wrong behavior. – Ethics is crucial in psychology when considering the treatment of research participants.

PsychologyThe scientific study of the mind and behavior, including the examination of mental processes and emotional responses. – Psychology provides insights into human behavior, which can inform ethical decision-making.

EmotionsComplex psychological states that involve subjective experiences, physiological responses, and behavioral expressions. – Understanding emotions is essential for philosophers and psychologists when analyzing human motivation and action.

Decision-makingThe cognitive process of selecting a course of action from multiple alternatives, often involving ethical considerations. – Decision-making in moral dilemmas requires careful evaluation of potential outcomes and ethical principles.

ConflictA situation in which incompatible desires, goals, or actions occur, often requiring resolution through ethical reasoning. – Philosophical discussions on conflict often explore how to balance competing interests and values.

HumanA member of the species Homo sapiens, characterized by complex cognitive abilities and social behaviors. – Philosophers and psychologists study what it means to be human, including our capacity for reason and emotion.

Well-beingA state of being comfortable, healthy, or happy, often considered in ethical theories as a goal of human action. – Utilitarianism evaluates actions based on their impact on the well-being of individuals and society as a whole.

All Video Lessons

Login your account

Please login your account to get started.

Don't have an account?

Register your account

Please sign up your account to get started.

Already have an account?